Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorBacso, Sarah A.
dc.contributor.authorNilsen, Elizabeth S.
dc.contributor.authorSilva, Janel
dc.date.accessioned2021-09-21 13:36:45 (GMT)
dc.date.available2021-09-21 13:36:45 (GMT)
dc.date.issued2021-07
dc.identifier.urihttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105097
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10012/17442
dc.descriptionThe final publication is available at Elsevier via http://dx.doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105097. © 2021. This manuscript version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/en
dc.description.abstractCommunication involves the integration of verbal and nonverbal cues. This study assessed preschool-age children’s ability to use their conversational partner’s facial expression to determine whether the partner required additional information or not. Children (aged 4;0–5;11 [years;months]; N = 101) played a game with a virtual child partner where they attempted to tell the virtual child in which box a prize was hidden. Children needed to provide several features of pictures on each box to uniquely identify the correct box. After providing their instructions, children viewed a video of the virtual child’s emotional reaction (prize found = happy, not found = sad). We assessed children’s recognition that miscommunication had occurred, their decision of whether or not to repair their message, and the content of their repairs. We found that children were able to determine whether or not the listener found the prize, and gauge their own skill at providing instructions, based on the listener’s facial expression. Furthermore, children were more likely to attempt to repair messages when the listener appeared to be sad, although their actual success in repairing the message was minimal. With respect to individual differences, children with higher executive functioning and higher emotion knowledge skills were more accurate in their perceptions of communicative success. Children with higher emotion knowledge skills were more likely to attempt to repair their messages when the listener appeared to be sad. Overall, this study demonstrates that children are able to make inferences about communication using a listener’s facial expression and that emotion recognition and executive functioning support this ability.en
dc.language.isoenen
dc.publisherElsevieren
dc.relation.ispartofseriesJournal of Experimental Child Psychology;
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/*
dc.subjectExecutive functioningen
dc.subjectEmotion recognitionen
dc.subjectEmotion understandingen
dc.subjectEmotion knowledgeen
dc.subjectCommunication repairen
dc.subjectReferential communicationen
dc.titleHow to turn that frown upside down: Children make use of a listener’s facial cues to detect and (attempt to) repair miscommunicationen
dc.typeArticleen
dcterms.bibliographicCitationBacso, S., Nilsen, E. S., & Silva, J. (2021). How to turn that frown upside down: Children make use of a listener’s facial cues to detect and (attempt to) repair miscommunication. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 207, 105097. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105097en
uws.contributor.affiliation1Faculty of Artsen
uws.contributor.affiliation2Psychologyen
uws.typeOfResourceTexten
uws.peerReviewStatusRevieweden
uws.scholarLevelFacultyen


Files in this item

Thumbnail
Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International

UWSpace

University of Waterloo Library
200 University Avenue West
Waterloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G1
519 888 4883

All items in UWSpace are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.

DSpace software

Service outages